There is safety in numbers.
Anyone who has ever had to walk home solo at night knows how vulnerable
we can be when alone. Animals
find safety in numbers too: animals often associate
into herds to reduce their probability of being attacked and to more easily
spot predators. But even though we know
that this happens, very little is known about the behavioural experiences that
contribute to the aggregation of individuals in response to predation.
Pollinators, like bees, also have predators, and have also evolved ways to avoid predation. Bees are threatened by ambush predators, like the crab spider (Family Thomisidae), which does not build webs, but wait on flowers to, you guessed it, ambush their bee prey. Some species of crab spiders are able to change their coloring, to camouflage themselves on flowers (below, left). Others sit on leaves, but look like inconspicuous bird droppings, tricking their prey into a false sense of security (below, right). But most crab spider ambushes are unsuccessful, and so bees are able to learn from their own experiences to avoid risky-looking flowers. But avoiding sketchy flowers also has the potential to reduce foraging efficiency, so why would this be an adaptive trait?
Pollinators, like bees, also have predators, and have also evolved ways to avoid predation. Bees are threatened by ambush predators, like the crab spider (Family Thomisidae), which does not build webs, but wait on flowers to, you guessed it, ambush their bee prey. Some species of crab spiders are able to change their coloring, to camouflage themselves on flowers (below, left). Others sit on leaves, but look like inconspicuous bird droppings, tricking their prey into a false sense of security (below, right). But most crab spider ambushes are unsuccessful, and so bees are able to learn from their own experiences to avoid risky-looking flowers. But avoiding sketchy flowers also has the potential to reduce foraging efficiency, so why would this be an adaptive trait?
Well, a new study examined this phenomenon. Bees use social cues to communicate between
individuals in a colony. For example, honeybees
use a “waggle dance” to share information about the direction and
distance to areas with lots of pollen and nectar, water sources, or new housing
locations. As it turns out, bumblebees
also use social cues, like the “lessons learned” of their bee friends, to
identify those flowers that are safe and those that are not.
Erika Dawson and Lars Chittka set up three bumblebee colonies in an artificial meadow where pollen sources were held in false flowers whose colors could be interchanged from white to yellow. Bees were encouraged to forage freely in order to familiarize themselves with the area, prior to being trained to recognize flower color with either a reward or a predation risk. One half of the bees were exposed to yellow flowers as the “safe” flowers, while the other half had white flowers as their “safe” flowers, to control for color preference. The “dangerous” flowers were equipped with foam-coated pincers that could rapidly close to trap, but not harm, bees that landed there. No other cues were provided other than flower color.
Erika Dawson and Lars Chittka set up three bumblebee colonies in an artificial meadow where pollen sources were held in false flowers whose colors could be interchanged from white to yellow. Bees were encouraged to forage freely in order to familiarize themselves with the area, prior to being trained to recognize flower color with either a reward or a predation risk. One half of the bees were exposed to yellow flowers as the “safe” flowers, while the other half had white flowers as their “safe” flowers, to control for color preference. The “dangerous” flowers were equipped with foam-coated pincers that could rapidly close to trap, but not harm, bees that landed there. No other cues were provided other than flower color.
No comments:
Post a Comment